1932

Abstract

This review uses a levels-of-analysis framework to summarize the current understanding of developmental dyslexia's etiology, brain bases, neuropsychology, and social context. Dyslexia is caused by multiple genetic and environmental risk factors as well as their interplay. Several candidate genes have been identified in the past decade. At the brain level, dyslexia is associated with aberrant structure and function, particularly in left hemisphere reading/language networks. The neurocognitive influences on dyslexia are also multifactorial and involve phonological processing deficits as well as weaknesses in other oral language skills and processing speed. We address contextual issues such as how dyslexia manifests across languages and social classes as well as what treatments are best supported. Throughout the review, we highlight exciting new research that cuts across levels of analysis. Such work promises eventually to provide a comprehensive explanation of the disorder as well as its prevention and remediation.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112842
2015-03-28
2024-04-18
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/clinpsy/11/1/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112842.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112842&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

Literature Cited

  1. Am. Psychiatr. Assoc 2000. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC: Am. Psychiatr. Publ, 4th. ed., text rev.
  2. Am. Psychiatr. Assoc 2013. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC: Am. Psychiatr. Publ, 5th ed..
  3. Aravena S, Snellings P, Tijms J, van der Molen MW. 2013. A lab-controlled simulation of a letter–speech sound binding deficit in dyslexia. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 115:691–707 [Google Scholar]
  4. Baker LA, Treloar SA, Reynolds CA, Heath AC, Martin NG. 1996. Genetics of educational attainment in Australian twins: sex differences and secular changes. Behav. Genet. 26:89–102 [Google Scholar]
  5. Berninger VW, Abbott RD, Thomson JB, Raskind WH. 2001. Language phenotype for reading and writing disability: a family approach. Sci. Stud. Read. 5:59–106 [Google Scholar]
  6. Bishop D, Rutter M. 2009. Neurodevelopmental disorders: conceptual issues. Rutter's Child and Adolescent Psychiatry M Rutter, DVM Bishop, DS Pine, S Scott, J Stevenson, E Taylor, A Thapar 32–41 Malden, MA: Blackwell Publ. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bishop DV, McDonald D, Bird S, Hayiou-Thomas ME. 2009. Children who read words accurately despite language impairment: Who are they and how do they do it?. Child Dev. 80:593–605 [Google Scholar]
  8. Blau V, Reithler J, van Atteveldt N, Seitz J, Gerretsen P. et al. 2010. Deviant processing of letters and speech sounds as proximate cause of reading failure: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of dyslexic children. Brain 133:868–79 [Google Scholar]
  9. Boada R, Pennington BF. 2006. Deficient implicit phonological representations in children with dyslexia. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 95:153–93 [Google Scholar]
  10. Boets B, de Beeck HPO, Vandermosten M, Scott SK, Gillebert CR. et al. 2013. Intact but less accessible phonetic representations in adults with dyslexia. Science 342:1251–54 [Google Scholar]
  11. Brown IS, Felton RH. 1990. Effects of instruction on beginning reading skills in children at risk for reading disability. Read. Writ. 2:223–41 [Google Scholar]
  12. Bruck M. 1992. Persistence of dyslexics' phonological deficits. Dev. Psychol. 28:874–86 [Google Scholar]
  13. Bruck M. 1993. Word recognition and component phonological processing skills of adults with childhood diagnosis of dyslexia. Dev. Rev. 13:258–68 [Google Scholar]
  14. Byrne B, Coventry WL, Olson RK, Samuelsson S, Corley R. et al. 2009. Genetic and environmental influences on aspects of literacy and language in early childhood: continuity and change from preschool to grade 2. J. Neurolinguistics 22:219–36 [Google Scholar]
  15. Caravolas M, Volín J, Hulme C. 2005. Phoneme awareness is a key component of alphabetic literacy skills in consistent and inconsistent orthographies: evidence from Czech and English children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 92:107–39 [Google Scholar]
  16. Carrion-Castillo A, Franke B, Fisher SE. 2013. Molecular genetics of dyslexia: an overview. Dyslexia 19:214–240Recent review of the molecular genetics of dyslexia. [Google Scholar]
  17. Castles A, Wilson K, Coltheart M. 2011. Early orthographic influences on phonemic awareness tasks: evidence from a preschool training study. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 108:203–10 [Google Scholar]
  18. Castro-Caldas A, Petersson KM, Reis A, Stone-Elander S, Ingvar M. 1998. The illiterate brain. Learning to read and write during childhood influences the functional organization of the adult brain. Brain 121:1053–63 [Google Scholar]
  19. Centanni TM, Booker AB, Sloan AM, Chen F, Maher B. et al. 2013. Knockdown of the dyslexia-associated gene Kiaa0319 impairs temporal responses to speech stimuli in rat primary auditory cortex. Cereb. Cortex 24:1753–66 [Google Scholar]
  20. Centanni TM, Chen F, Booker AM, Engineer CT, Sloan AM. et al. 2014. Speech sound processing deficits and training-induced neural plasticity in rats with dyslexia gene knockdown. PLOS ONE 9:e98439 [Google Scholar]
  21. Chomsky N, Halle M. 1968. The Sound Pattern of English New York: Harper & Row
  22. Christopher ME, Hulslander J, Byrne B, Samuelsson S, Keenan JM. et al. 2013. The genetic and environmental etiologies of individual differences in early reading growth in Australia, the United States, and Scandinavia. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 115:453–67 [Google Scholar]
  23. Christopher ME, Miyake A, Keenan JM, Pennington B, DeFries JC. et al. 2012. Predicting word reading and comprehension with executive function and speed measures across development: a latent variable analysis. J. Exp. Psychol: Gen. 141:470–88 [Google Scholar]
  24. Cunningham AE, Stanovich KE. 1998. The impact of print exposure on word recognition. Word Recognition in Beginning Literacy JL Metsala, LC Ehri 235–62 Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
  25. Darki F, Peyrard-Janvid M, Matsson H, Kere J, Klingberg T. 2012. Three dyslexia susceptibility genes, DYX1C1, DCDC2, and KIAA0319, affect temporo-parietal white matter structure. Biol. Psychiatry 72:671–76One of the few existing studies linking dyslexia candidate genes to brain structure. [Google Scholar]
  26. Demonet JF, Taylor MJ, Chaix Y. 2004. Developmental dyslexia. Lancet 363:1451–60 [Google Scholar]
  27. Deutsch GK, Dougherty RF, Bammer R, Siok WT, Gabrieli JDE, Wandell B. 2005. Children's reading performance is correlated with white matter structure measured by tensor imaging. Cortex 41:354–63 [Google Scholar]
  28. Dickie C, Ota M, Clark A. 2013. Revisiting the phonological deficit in dyslexia: Are implicit nonorthographic representations impaired?. Appl. Psychol. 34:649–72 [Google Scholar]
  29. Elbro C, Borstrom I, Petersen D. 1998. Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten: the importance of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items. Read. Res. Q. 33:36–60 [Google Scholar]
  30. Facoetti A, Corradi N, Ruffino M, Gori S, Zorzi M. 2010. Visual spatial attention and speech segmentation are both impaired in preschoolers at familial risk for developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia 16:226–39 [Google Scholar]
  31. Fisher SE, DeFries JC. 2002. Developmental dyslexia: genetic dissection of a complex cognitive trait. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3:767–80 [Google Scholar]
  32. Fowler AE. 1991. How early phonological development might set the stage for phoneme awareness. Phonological Processes in Literacy: A Tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman SA Brady, DP Shankweiler 97–117 Mahwah, NJ: ErlbaumSeminal chapter arguing for developmental progression in phonological representations themselves, not just in meta-linguistic awareness. [Google Scholar]
  33. Franceschini S, Gori S, Ruffino M, Pedroll K, Facoetti A. 2012. A causal link between spatial attention and reading acquisition. Curr. Biol. 22:814–19 [Google Scholar]
  34. Friend A, DeFries JC, Olson RK. 2008. Parental education moderates genetic influences on reading disability. Psychol. Sci. 19:1124–30 [Google Scholar]
  35. Gabrieli JD. 2009. Dyslexia: a new synergy between education and cognitive neuroscience. Science 325:280–83 [Google Scholar]
  36. Galaburda AM, Sherman GF, Rosen GD, Aboitiz F, Geschwind N. 1985. Developmental dyslexia: four consecutive patients with cortical anomalies. Ann. Neurol. 18:222–33 [Google Scholar]
  37. Gialluisi A, Newbury DF, Wilcutt EG, Olson RK, DeFries JC. et al. 2014. Genome-wide screening for DNA variants associated with reading and language traits. Genes Brain Behav. 13:686–701 [Google Scholar]
  38. Goldinger SD, Azuma T. 2003. Puzzle-solving science: the quixotic quest for units in speech perception. J. Phonet. 31:305–20 [Google Scholar]
  39. Greenberg S. 2004. A multi-tier framework for understanding spoken language. Listening to Speech: An Auditory Perspective S Greenberg, W Ainsworth 411–33 Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
  40. Guttorm TK, Leppanen PH, Poikkeus AM, Eklund KM, Lyytinen P, Lyytinen H. 2005. Brain event-related potentials (ERPs) measured at birth predict later language development in children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Cortex 41:291–303 [Google Scholar]
  41. Harlaar N, Butcher LM, Meaburn E, Sham P, Craig IW, Plomin R. 2005. A behavioural genomic analysis of DNA markers associated with general cognitive ability in 7-year-olds. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 46:1097–107 [Google Scholar]
  42. Hatcher J, Snowling MJ, Griffiths YM. 2002. Cognitive assessment of dyslexic students in higher education. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 72:119–33 [Google Scholar]
  43. Hayiou-Thomas ME, Dale PS, Plomin R. 2012. The etiology of variation in language skills changes with development: a longitudinal twin study of language from 2 to 12 years. Dev. Sci. 15:233–49 [Google Scholar]
  44. Hecht SA, Burgess SR, Torgesen JK, Wagner RK, Rashotte CA. 2000. Explaining social class differences in growth of reading skills from beginning kindergarten through fourth-grade: the role of phonological awareness, rate of access, and print knowledge. Read. Writ. 12:99–127 [Google Scholar]
  45. Hensler BS, Schatschneider C, Taylor J, Wagner RK. 2010. Behavioral genetic approach to the study of dyslexia. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 31:525–32 [Google Scholar]
  46. Hoff E. 2003. Causes and consequences of SES-related differences in parent-to-child speech. Socioeconomic Status, Parenting, and Child Development. Monographs in Parenting Series MH Bornsten, RH Bradley 147–60 Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
  47. Hoover WA, Gough PB. 1990. The simple view of reading. Read. Writ. 2:127–60Classic paper proposing that reading comprehension is a function of decoding and oral language skill. [Google Scholar]
  48. Hu W, Lee HL, Zhang Q, Liu T, Geng LB. et al. 2010. Developmental dyslexia in Chinese and English populations: dissociating the effect of dyslexia from language differences. Brain 133:1694–706 [Google Scholar]
  49. Hulme C, Bowyer-Crane C, Carroll JM, Duff FJ, Snowling MJ. 2012. The causal role of phoneme awareness and letter-sound knowledge in learning to read combining intervention studies with mediation analyses. Psychol. Sci. 23:572–77 [Google Scholar]
  50. Jimenez JE, Siegel L, O'Shanahan I, Ford L. 2009. The relative roles of IQ and cognitive processes in reading disability. Educ. Psychol. 29:27–43 [Google Scholar]
  51. Joanisse MF, Manis FR, Keating P, Seidenberg MS. 2000. Language deficits in dyslexic children: speech perception, phonology, and morphology. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 77:30–60 [Google Scholar]
  52. Kere J. 2011. Molecular genetics and molecular biology of dyslexia. WIREs Cogn. Sci. 2:441–48 [Google Scholar]
  53. Kerr J. 1897. School hygiene, in its mental, moral, and physical aspects. Howard Medical Prize Essay. J. R. Stat. Soc. 60:613–80 [Google Scholar]
  54. Klingberg T, Hedehus M, Temple E, Salz T, Gabrieli JD. et al. 2000. Microstructure of temporo-parietal white matter as a basis for reading ability: evidence from diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging. Neuron 25:493–500First empirical demonstration of the association between left hemisphere white matter integrity and reading. [Google Scholar]
  55. Landerl K, Moll K. 2010. Comorbidity of learning disorders: prevalence and familial transmission. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 51:287–94 [Google Scholar]
  56. Landerl K, Wimmer H, Frith U. 1997. The impact of orthographic consistency on dyslexia: a German-English comparison. Cognition 63:315–34 [Google Scholar]
  57. Liberman IY. 1973. Segmentation of the spoken word and reading acquisition. Ann. Dyslexia 23:64–77 [Google Scholar]
  58. Logan JAR, Cutting L, Schatschneider C, Hart SA, Deater-Deckard K, Peterill S. 2013. Reading development in young children: genetic and environmental influence. Child Dev. 84:2131–44 [Google Scholar]
  59. Lonigan CJ, Whitehurst GJ. 1998. Relative efficacy of parent and teacher involvement in a shared-reading intervention for preschool children from low-income backgrounds. Early Child. Res. Q. 13:263–90 [Google Scholar]
  60. Ludwig KU, Schumacher J, Schulte-Korne G, Konig IR, Warnke A. et al. 2008. Investigation of the DCDC2 intron 2 deletion/compound short tandem repeat polymorphism in a large German dyslexia sample. Psychiatry Genet. 18:310–12 [Google Scholar]
  61. Lynn R, Hampson S. 1986. The rise of national intelligence: evidence from Britain, Japan and the USA. Personal. Individ. Differ. 7:23–32 [Google Scholar]
  62. Lyon GR, Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA. 2003. A definition of dyslexia. Ann. Dyslexia 53:1–14 [Google Scholar]
  63. MacDonald Wer B. 2014. Comparison of reading development across socioeconomic status in the United States PhD Thesis, Univ. Denver, CO
  64. Manis FR, McBride-Chang C, Seidenberg MS, Keating P. 1997. Are speech perception deficits associated with developmental dyslexia?. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 66:211–35 [Google Scholar]
  65. Mathes PG, Denton CA, Fletcher JM, Anthony JL, Francis DJ, Schatschneider C. 2005. The effects of theoretically different instruction and student characteristics on the skills of struggling readers. Read. Res. Q. 40:148–82 [Google Scholar]
  66. McBride-Chang C, Cho J-R, Liu H, Wagner RK, Shu H. et al. 2005. Changing models across cultures: associations of phonological awareness and morphological structure awareness with vocabulary and word recognition in second graders from Beijing, Hong Kong, Korea, and the United States. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 92:140–60 [Google Scholar]
  67. McGrath L, Pennington BF, Shanahan MA, Santerre-Lemmon LE, Barnard HD. et al. 2011. A multiple deficit model of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: searching for shared cognitive deficits. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 52:547–57 [Google Scholar]
  68. McGrath L, Smith SD, Pennington BF. 2006. Breakthroughs in the search for dyslexia candidate genes. Trends Mol. Med. 12:333–41 [Google Scholar]
  69. Meaburn EL, Harlaar N, Craig IW, Schalkwyk LC, Plomin R. 2008. Quantitative trait locus association scan of early reading disability and ability using pooled DNA and 100K SNP microarrays in a sample of 5760 children. Mol. Psychiatry 13:729–40 [Google Scholar]
  70. Metsala JL, Walley AC. 1998. Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. Word Recognition in Beginning Literacy JL Metsala, LC Ehri 89–120 Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
  71. Morais J, Cary L, Alegria J, Bertelson P. 1979. Does awareness of speech as a sequence of phones arise spontaneously?. Cognition 7:323–31Seminal paper demonstrating that becoming literate in an alphabetic language increases phoneme awareness. [Google Scholar]
  72. Mundy IR, Carroll JM. 2012. Speech prosody and developmental dyslexia: reduced phonological awareness in the context of intact phonological representations. J. Cogn. Psychol. 24:560–81 [Google Scholar]
  73. Nation K, Cocksey J, Taylor JS, Bishop DV. 2010. A longitudinal investigation of early reading and language skills in children with poor reading comprehension. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 51:1031–39 [Google Scholar]
  74. Natl. Cent. Educ. Stat 2011. The Nation's Report Card: Reading 2011 NCES 2012-457 Washington, DC: US Dep. Educ., Inst. Educ. Sci.
  75. Natl. Read. Panel 2000. Teaching children to read: an evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. Reports of the subgroups. NIH Publ. No. 00-4754 Washington, DC: US GPO
  76. Nittrouer S, Pennington BF. 2010. New approaches to the study of childhood language disorders. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 19:308–13 [Google Scholar]
  77. Orton ST. 1925. “Word-blindness” in school children. Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 14:581–615 [Google Scholar]
  78. Orton ST. 1937. Reading, Writing and Speech Problems in Children New York: Norton
  79. Paulesu E, Demonet JF, Fazio F, McCrory E, Chanoine V. et al. 2001. Dyslexia: cultural diversity and biological unity. Science 291:2165–67Elegant demonstration that cross-cultural universality exists in brain bases of dyslexia despite variability in its manifestation. [Google Scholar]
  80. Pennington BF. 2002. The Development of Psychopathology: Nature and Nurture New York: Guilford
  81. Pennington BF. 2006. From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition 101:385 [Google Scholar]
  82. Pennington BF. 2011. Controversial therapies for dyslexia. Perspect. Lang. Literacy: Q. Publ. Int. Dyslexia Assoc. 37: [Google Scholar]
  83. Pennington BF, Bishop DVM. 2009. Relations among speech, language, and reading disorders. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60:283–306 [Google Scholar]
  84. Pennington BF, Lefly DL. 2001. Early reading development in children at family risk for dyslexia. Child Dev. 72:816–33 [Google Scholar]
  85. Pennington BF, Olson RK. 2005. Genetics of dyslexia. The Science of Reading: A Handbook M Snowling, C Hulme 453–72 Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publ. [Google Scholar]
  86. Pennington BF, Santerre-Lemmon L, Rosenberg J, MacDonald B, Boada R. et al. 2012. Individual prediction of dyslexia by single versus multiple deficit models. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 121:212–24 [Google Scholar]
  87. Perfetti CA, Zhang S, Berent I. 1992. Reading in English and Chinese: evidence for a “universal” phonological principle. Adv. Psychol. 94:227–48 [Google Scholar]
  88. Peterson RL, Pennington BF, Shriberg LD, Boada R. 2009. What influences literacy outcome in children with speech sound disorder?. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 52:1175–88 [Google Scholar]
  89. Petrill SA, Deater-Deckard K, Schatschneider C, Davis C. 2005. Measured environmental influences on early reading: evidence from an adoption study. Sci. Stud. Read. 9:237–59 [Google Scholar]
  90. Phillips BM, Lonigan CJ. 2009. Variations in the home literacy environment of preschool children: a cluster analytic approach. Sci. Stud. Read. 13:146–74 [Google Scholar]
  91. Platt M, Adler W, Mehlhorn A, Johnson G, Wright K. et al. 2013. Embryonic disruption of the candidate dyslexia susceptibility gene homolog Kiaa0319 results in neuronal migration disorders. Neuroscience 248:585–93 [Google Scholar]
  92. Plaut DC, Kello CT. 1999. The emergence of phonology from the interplay of speech comprehension and production: a distributed connectionist approach. The Emergence of Language B MacWhinney 381–415 Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum [Google Scholar]
  93. Plomin R, Haworth CM, Meaburn EL, Price TS, Davis OS. 2013. Common DNA markers can account for more than half of the genetic influence on cognitive abilities. Psychol. Sci. 24:562–68 [Google Scholar]
  94. Pollatsek A. 2014. The role of phonological coding in silent reading of words and texts. Oxford Handbook of Reading A Pollatsek, R Treiman Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press. In press [Google Scholar]
  95. Port R. 2007. How are words stored in memory? Beyond phones and phonemes. New Ideas Psychol. 25:143–70 [Google Scholar]
  96. Pringle-Morgan W. 1896. A case of congenital word blindness. Br. Med. J. 2:1543–44 [Google Scholar]
  97. Puolakanaho A, Ahonen T, Aro M, Eklund K, Leppanen PHT. et al. 2008. Developmental links of very early phonological and language skills to second grade reading outcomes: strong to accuracy but only minor to fluency. J. Learn. Disabil. 41:353–70 [Google Scholar]
  98. Ramus F. 2003. Developmental dyslexia: specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction?. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 13:212–18 [Google Scholar]
  99. Ramus F. 2014. Neuroimaging sheds new light on the phonological deficit in dyslexia. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18:274–75 [Google Scholar]
  100. Ramus F, Szenkovits G. 2008. What phonological deficit?. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 61:129–41 [Google Scholar]
  101. Raschle NM, Chang M, Gaab N. 2011. Structural brain alterations associated with dyslexia predate reading onset. NeuroImage 57:742–49 [Google Scholar]
  102. Richlan F, Kronbichler M, Wimmer H. 2009. Functional abnormalities in the dyslexic brain: a quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30:3299–308Recent meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies in dyslexia. [Google Scholar]
  103. Rimrodt SL, Peterson DJ, Denckla MB, Kaufmann WE, Cutting LE. 2010. White matter microstructural differences linked to left perisylvian language network in children with dyslexia. Cortex 46:739–49 [Google Scholar]
  104. Robins S, Ghosh D, Rosales N, Treiman R. 2014. Letter knowledge in parent-child conversations: differences between families differing in socio-economic status. Front. Psychol. 5:632 [Google Scholar]
  105. Rodgers B. 1983. The identification and prevalence of specific reading retardation. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 53:369–73 [Google Scholar]
  106. Rutter M, Caspi A, Fergusson D, Horwood LJ, Goodman R. et al. 2004. Sex differences in developmental reading disability: new findings from 4 epidemiological studies. JAMA 291:2007–12 [Google Scholar]
  107. Scammacca N, Vaughn S, Roberts G, Wanzek J, Torgesen JK. 2007. Extensive Reading Interventions in Grades K-3: From Research to Practice Portsmouth, NH: RMC Res. Corp., Cent. Instr.Overview of evidence-based interventions for dyslexia.
  108. Scarborough HS. 1990. Very early language deficits in dyslexic children. Child Dev. 61:1728–43Early demonstration, since replicated many times, of oral language deficits in predyslexic children. [Google Scholar]
  109. Scarborough HS. 1991. Antecedents to reading disability: preschool language development and literacy experiences of children from dyslexic families. Read. Writ. 3:219–33 [Google Scholar]
  110. Scarborough HS. 1998. Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities: phonological awareness and some other promising predictors. Specific Reading Disability: A View of the Spectrum BK Shapiro, PJ Accardo, AJ Capute 75–119 Timonium, MD: York Press [Google Scholar]
  111. Scarborough HS, Dobrich W. 1994. On the efficacy of reading to preschoolers. Dev. Rev. 14:245–302 [Google Scholar]
  112. Scarborough HS, Dobrich W, Hager M. 1991. Preschool literacy experience and later reading achievement. J. Learn. Disabil. 24:508–11 [Google Scholar]
  113. Scarr S, McCartney K. 1983. How people make their own environments: a theory of genotype-environment effects. Child Dev. 54:424–35 [Google Scholar]
  114. Scerri TS, Morris AP, Buckingham LL, Newbury DF, Miller LL. et al. 2011. DCDC2, KIAA0319 and CMIP are associated with reading-related traits. Biol. Psychiatry 70:237–45 [Google Scholar]
  115. Scerri TS, Paracchini S, Morris A, MacPhie IL, Talcott J. et al. 2010. Identification of candidate genes for dyslexia susceptibility on chromosome 18. PLOS ONE 5:e13712 [Google Scholar]
  116. Sénéchal M. 2014. Young children's home literacy experiences. Oxford Handbook of Reading A Pollatsek, R Treiman Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press. In press [Google Scholar]
  117. Shaywitz SE, Escobar MD, Shaywitz BA, Fletcher JM, Makuch R. 1992. Evidence that dyslexia may represent the lower tail of a normal distribution of reading ability. N. Engl. J. Med. 326:145–50 [Google Scholar]
  118. Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA, Fletcher JM, Escobar MD. 1990. Prevalence of reading disability in boys and girls. Results of the Connecticut Longitudinal Study. JAMA 264:998–1002 [Google Scholar]
  119. Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA, Fulbright RK, Skudlarski P, Mencl WE. et al. 2003. Neural systems for compensation and persistence: young adult outcome of childhood reading disability. Biol. Psychiatry 54:25–33 [Google Scholar]
  120. Silani G, Frith U, Demonet JF, Fazio F, Perani D. et al. 2005. Brain abnormalities underlying altered activation in dyslexia: a voxel based morphometry study. Brain 128:2453–61 [Google Scholar]
  121. Sirin SR. 2005. Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: a meta-analytic review of research. Rev. Educ. Res. 75:417–53 [Google Scholar]
  122. Smith SD, Gilger JW, Pennington BF. 2001. Dyslexia and other specific learning disorders. Emery and Rimoin's Principles and Practice of Medical Genetics DL Rimoin, JM Conner, RE Pyeritz 2827–65 New York: Churchill Livingstone, 4th ed.. [Google Scholar]
  123. Snow CE, Burns S, Griffin P. 1998. Preventing Reading Difficulties in Young Children Washington, DC: Natl. Acad. Press
  124. Snowling MJ, Gallagher A, Frith U. 2003. Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. Child Dev. 74:358–73 [Google Scholar]
  125. Snowling MJ, Hulme C. 2011. Evidence-based interventions for reading and language difficulties: creating a virtuous circle. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 81:1–23 [Google Scholar]
  126. Stanovich KE. 1986. Matthew effects in reading: some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Read. Res. Q. 21:360–406 [Google Scholar]
  127. Stuebing KK, Barth AE, Molfese PJ, Weiss B, Fletcher JM. 2009. IQ is not strongly related to response to reading instruction: a meta-analytic interpretation. Except. Child. 76:31–51 [Google Scholar]
  128. Sylva K, Scott S, Totsika V, Ereky-Stevens K, Crook C. 2008. Training parents to help their children read: a randomized control trial. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 78:435–55 [Google Scholar]
  129. Szalkowski CE, Fiondella CF, Truong DT, Rosen GD, LoTurco JJ, Fitch RH. 2013. The effects of Kiaa0319 knockdown on cortical and subcortical anatomy in male rats. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 31:116–22 [Google Scholar]
  130. Torgesen JK. 2005. Recent discoveries on remedial interventions for children with dyslexia. The Science of Reading: A Handbook MJ Snowling, C Hulme 521–37 Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publ. [Google Scholar]
  131. Torppa M, Lyytinen P, Erskine J, Eklund K, Lyytinen H. 2010. Language development, literacy skills, and predictive connections to reading in Finnish children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. J. Learn. Disabil. 43:308–21 [Google Scholar]
  132. Treiman R. 1985. Onsets and rimes as units of spoken syllables: evidence from children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 39:161–81 [Google Scholar]
  133. Vaughn S, Cirino PT, Wanzek J, Wexler J, Fletcher JM. et al. 2010. Response to intervention for middle school students with reading difficulties: effects of a primary and secondary intervention. School Psychol. Rev. 39:3–21 [Google Scholar]
  134. Vaughn S, Linan-Thompson S, Kouzekanani K, Bryant DP, Dickson S, Blozis SA. 2003. Reading instruction grouping for students with reading difficulties. Remedial Spec. Educ. 24:301–15 [Google Scholar]
  135. Vellutino FR. 1979. The validity of perceptual deficit explanations of reading disability: a reply to Fletcher and Satz. J. Learn. Disabil. 12:160–67 [Google Scholar]
  136. Vellutino FR, Fletcher JM, Snowling MJ, Scanlon DM. 2004. Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades?. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 45:2–40 [Google Scholar]
  137. Vellutino FR, Scanlon DM, Small SG, Fanuele DP. 2006. Response to intervention as a vehicle for distinguishing between children with and without reading disabilities: evidence for the role of kindergarten and first-grade interventions. J. Learn. Disabil. 39:157–69 [Google Scholar]
  138. Wadsworth SJ, Corley RP, Hewitt JK, DeFries JC. 2001. Stability of genetic and environmental influences on reading performance at 7, 12, and 16 years of age in the Colorado Adoption Project. Behav. Genet. 31:353–59 [Google Scholar]
  139. Wadsworth SJ, Olson RK, DeFries JC. 2010. Differential genetic etiology of reading difficulties as a function of IQ: an update. Behav. Genet. 40:751–58 [Google Scholar]
  140. Wagner RK, Torgesen JK. 1987. The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychol. Bull. 101:192–212 [Google Scholar]
  141. White KR. 1982. The relation between socioeconomic status and academic achievement. Psychol. Bull. 91:461–81 [Google Scholar]
  142. Willcutt EG, Pennington BF. 2000. Psychiatric comorbidity in children and adolescents with reading disability. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 41:1039–48 [Google Scholar]
  143. Willcutt EG, Pennington BF, Duncan L, Smith SD, Keenan JM. et al. 2010. Understanding the complex etiologies of developmental disorders: behavioral and molecular genetic approaches. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 31:533–44 [Google Scholar]
  144. Wolf M, Bowers PG. 1999. The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. J. Educ. Psychol. 91:415–38 [Google Scholar]
  145. Ziegler JC, Bertrand D, Toth D, Csepe V, Reis A. et al. 2010. Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading: a cross-language investigation. Psychol. Sci. 21:551–59 [Google Scholar]
  146. Zorzi M, Barbiero C, Facoetti A, Lonciari I, Carrozzi M. et al. 2012. Extra-large letter spacing improves reading in dyslexia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 109:11455–59 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112842
Loading
/content/journals/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112842
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Review Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was a Success
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error